Short men are indirectly aggressive toward taller men, study finds
The study shows when the 'Napoleon complex' is most likely to emerge.
02 January, 2019
Artist: Andrea Appiani
- A recent study examined the Napoleon complex through economic games.
- The results showed that shorter men are more likely than taller men to keep a disproportionate amount of resources for themselves, but only when the other player can't retaliate.
- The study suggests that the Napoleon complex is most likely to manifest in situations where the shorter man has all the power.
</li></ul>
<p>In the early 19th century, Napoléon Bonaparte was perhaps best known for leading successful military campaigns and serving as the Emperor of the French for nearly a decade. But today, the ruthless French leader is probably best remembered in the popular imagination for his short height, a trait that inspired what many now call the Napoleon complex.<br></p><p>The Napoleon complex is a popular belief that describes an inferiority complex in which short men tend to compensate for their small stature through behavior, such as increased aggression or gossiping. From an evolutionary perspective, it makes sense that short men might try to compensate; research shows that tall men are <a href="https://www.theatlantic.com/business/archive/2015/05/the-financial-perks-of-being-tall/393518/" target="_blank">more likely to hold positions of power</a>, attract mates and be perceived as <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797618772822#_i23" target="_blank">higher status by their peers</a>.</p><p>A new study published in the journal <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797618772822" target="_blank"><em>Psychological Science</em></a> uses economic games to examine the Napoleon complex, providing some of the first results on the importance of height in competition between men.</p><p>In an economic experiment called the dictator game, participants were asked to divide a sum of money between themselves and an unseen opponent. Each participant could divvy up the money however he wished. Interestingly, the participants who tended to keep the most money for themselves in this version of the game weren't necessarily shorter—they were people who reported that they often <em>felt small.</em></p>
<img type="lazy-image" data-runner-src="https://assets.rebelmouse.io/eyJhbGciOiJIUzI1NiIsInR5cCI6IkpXVCJ9.eyJpbWFnZSI6Imh0dHBzOi8vYXNzZXRzLnJibC5tcy8xOTA1NDQxNC9vcmlnaW4ucG5nIiwiZXhwaXJlc19hdCI6MTY1MDY5MTU5OH0.2F0br50XB6Z8Mq5QaYXWvdPvCMviheeNFl-XHPN0NRA/img.png?width=980" id="65864" class="rm-shortcode" data-rm-shortcode-id="8d12e01443e1240176267e0f8a00f11a" data-rm-shortcode-name="rebelmouse-image" /><p>The researchers then conducted the same game in a competitive setting, in which two male opponents met face to face, had their heights recorded and read aloud (along with other physical and strength measurements), and were asked to enter separate cubicles. Again, the participants had to divide a sum of money. The allocator could choose to give any amount, or nothing, to his opponent, who was the receiver. The researchers told the participants that one person would play the allocator and the other would play the receiver, but in reality every participant played the allocator.<br></p><p>The results showed that, on average, relatively shorter men kept more money for themselves.</p><p>Next, the participants played an ultimatum game in which an allocator divides a sum of money, keeping some of it and offering a portion of his choice to the receiver. But if the receiver perceives the offer to be unfair, he can reject it and both parties get nothing. </p><p>Unlike the dictator game, height didn't seem to play a significant role in influencing how much money participants chose to keep in the ultimatum game.</p><p>In another experiment, two male opponents once again played a dictator game. This time, however, each participant also had to choose an amount of hot sauce their opponent would have to consume, which was, in theory, a measure of aggression. But the results showed that shorter men were not significantly more likely to make their opponents eat more hot sauce.</p>
Men show flexible behavior in competitions
<p>The study suggests that shorter men are more likely to show indirect, rather than direct, aggression toward taller men in competitions for resources. For shorter men, the researchers wrote that these indirect strategies represent safer options than physical combat. Also, the results suggest that the Napoleon complex is most likely to manifest in situations where the shorter man has all the power, and the taller man can't retaliate.</p><p>"The results imply that participant height is most important in predicting competitive behaviors in an absolute-power situation (the dictator game), regardless of opponent height," the researchers wrote. "This is not surprising as shorter and taller men likely have different life experiences that may influence their decision making in behavioral experiments."<br></p><p>It's not just competitions between men that bring out the Napoleon complex, the researchers noted.</p><p>"In terms of underlying mechanisms, the Napoleon complex may also be shaped by intersexual selection forces—shorter men could use behavioral strategies to impress females, such as risk taking, generosity, or showing commitment (e.g., <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797618772822#" target="_blank">Griskevicius et al., 2007</a>; <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797618772822#" target="_blank">Iredale, Van Vugt, & Dunbar, 2008</a>)."</p><p>The researchers suggested it'd be interesting to see whether men would behave differently in these kinds of economic games if an attractive female were also involved.</p><p>"For further studies, it would be of great interest to add a potential mating opportunity to the paradigm to see how intersexual competition affects the Napoleon complex. The presence of an attractive female could exacerbate other kinds of overcompensating behaviors in short men—for example, an increased propensity toward risk taking to impress women."</p><p><em>The study, "<a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797618772822" target="_blank">The Napoleon Complex: When Shorter Men Take More</a>", was authored by Jill E. P. Knapen, Nancy M. Blaker, and Mark Van Vugt.</em></p>
Keep reading
Show less
The Real Story of What Got Us to the Top of the Food Chain
Natural "narrative selection" was key to turning insignificant apes (who had tools for 2 million years) into the species that now dominates the bio-sphere.
09 March, 2017
<p dir="ltr"><span>1. What got us to the top of the food chain? Yuval Harari says it wasn’t bigger brains and tools. His view of what mattered will surprise fans of evolution’s red-in-tooth-and-claw story.</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>2. Going from “stone-tipped spears to… </span><a href="https://www.google.com/search?tbm=bks&hl=en&q=yuval+harari+%22homo+deus%22+%22hunting+mammoth%22#hl=en&tbm=bks&q=yuval+harari+%22homo+deus%22+Over+those+20,000+years+humankind+moved+from+hunting+mammoth+with+stonetipped+spears+to+exploring+the+solar+system+with+spaceships+&*" target="_blank"><span>spaceships</span></a><span>” took us ~20,000 years; meanwhile, our brains have </span><a href="https://www.google.com/search?tbm=bks&hl=en&q=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22bigger+brains%22#hl=en&tbm=bks&q=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22+not+thanks+to+the+evolution+of+more+dexterous+hands+or+bigger+brains+(our+brains+today+seem+actually+to+be+smaller)&*" target="_blank"><span>shrunk</span></a><span> ~10% (note: Neanderthal brains were </span><a href="http://pllqt.it/cTb3U2" target="_blank"><span>bigger</span></a><span>). </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>3. But we’ve had tools for ~2 million years (intelligently designed tools have long shaped our </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/our-tools-have-changed-our-genes-for-millions-of-years" target="_blank"><span>genes</span></a><span>).</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>4. Harari says the “crucial </span><a href="https://www.google.com/search?tbm=bks&hl=en&q=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22the+crucial+factor%22#hl=en&tbm=bks&q=yuval+harari+homo+deus+the+crucial+factor+in+our+conquest+of+the+world+was+our+ability+to+connect+many+humans+to+one+another.&*" target="_blank"><span>factor</span></a><span>” was our unique capacity “to cooperate flexibly in large </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=DczADQAAQBAJ&pg=PP116&dq=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22homo+sapiens+is+the+only+species%22&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjgx6qip8XSAhUmM8AKHcihDHoQ6AEIHDAA#v=onepage&q=Homo%20sapiens%20is%20the%20only%20species%20on%20earth%20capable%20of%20cooperating%20flexibly%20in%20large%20numbers%22&f=false" target="_blank"><span>numbers</span></a><span>.”</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>5. Cooperation is critical for both types of what biologist E. O. Wilson calls “the social conquest of </span><a href="http://www.nytimes.com/2012/05/13/books/review/the-social-conquest-of-earth-by-edward-o-wilson.html" target="_blank"><span>Earth</span></a><span>.” Humans and social insects dominate the biosphere (because, organized groups can always outperform individuals, in combat, and in peacetime productivity). </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>6. Ants and bees were doing large-scale cooperation millions of years before us. But their cooperation is kin-based and </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=DczADQAAQBAJ&pg=PP116&dq=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22ants+and+bees%22&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwi5v5yTrMXSAhUJCsAKHRgKBewQ6AEIHDAA#v=snippet&q=%22ants%20and%20bees%22%20%22learned%20to%20cooperate%20en%20masse%20millions%20of%20years%20before%20us%22%20their%20cooperation%20lacks%20flexibility&f=false" target="_blank"><span>inflexible</span></a><span> (adapting genetically = slowly). </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>7. David Sloan Wilson calls teamwork humanity’s “signature </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/how-evolutioneconomics-have-an-equivalent-inclusive-fitness-in-evolution-and" target="_blank"><span>adaptation</span></a><span>,” but Harari describes how scaling beyond team-level cooperation was key. </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>8. This “large-scale human cooperation” requires shared </span><a href="https://www.google.com/search?tbm=bks&hl=en&q=yuval+harari+%22homo+deus%22+no+complex+human+society+can+function+without+commonly+accepted+stories." target="_blank"><span>stories</span></a><span>, because “The human mind is a story processor, not a logic </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=U21BxGfm3RUC&printsec=frontcover#v=onepage&q=%22human%20mind%20is%20a%20story%20processor%2C%20not%20a%20logic%20processor%22&f=false" target="_blank"><span>processor</span></a><span>." Stories transmit what matters in a culture, and configure our “emotional </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/our-storied-nature" target="_blank"><span>grammar</span></a><span>” (aside: Harari usefully calls emotions “biochemical </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=DczADQAAQBAJ&pg=PP305&dq=yuval+harari+%22homo+deus%22+biochemical+algorithms&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjWwobX2snSAhWJ6oMKHdfLCWAQ6AEIIjAB#v=snippet&q=%22thoughts%20emotions%20and%20sensations%20are%22%20%22biochemical%20algorithms%22&f=false" target="_blank"><span>algorithms</span></a><span>”).</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>10. We live in a “web of </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=DczADQAAQBAJ&pg=PP305&dq=yuval+harari+%22homo+deus%22+biochemical+algorithms&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjWwobX2snSAhWJ6oMKHdfLCWAQ6AEIIjAB#v=snippet&q=%22web%20of%20stories%22&f=false" target="_blank"><span>stories</span></a><span>” about what matters. In that sense </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/beauty-and-duty-now-often-clash-how-art-works-on-hidden-brain-system" target="_blank"><span>artists</span></a><span> and “storytellers run the </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/what-stories-run-your-world" target="_blank"><span>world</span></a><span>,” and a culture’s storytelling resources shape its politics and </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=00rsK2Y98gQC&printsec=frontcover&dq=macintyre+after+virtue&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjFq6aL1ufQAhXE6CYKHVpOC7UQ6wEIGzAA#v=onepage&q=the%20chief%20means%20of%20moral%20education%20is%20the%20telling%20of%20%20stories&f=false" target="_blank"><span>moralities</span></a><span> (Alasdair </span><a href="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Alasdair_MacIntyre" target="_blank"><span>MacIntyre</span></a><span>). </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>11. Languages, </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/our-storied-nature" target="_blank"><span>stories</span></a><span>, the arts, religions, </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/moral-sciences-are-back" target="_blank"><span>moralities</span></a><span>, </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/what-the-most-successful-euphemism-in-history-is-doing-to-us" target="_blank"><span>politics</span></a><span>, and </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/how-paleo-economics-shaped-us-physically-and-morally" target="_blank"><span>economics</span></a><span> are all social tools for organizing cooperation. </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>12. We’d be wiser to call ourselves “Homo Storius” or “Homo Narratus” or "Homo Socius" rather than Homo Sapiens (sapiens derives from judgement or </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=Ast1CwAAQBAJ&pg=PP1&dq=becoming+wise&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwi1jMb1iunMAhVBHR4KHeSHA_8Q6wEIUDAH#v=onepage&q=connection%20%22root%20of%20the%20word%20sapiens%22%20%22knowledge%20and%20wisdom%20and%20taste%22%20&f=false" target="_blank"><span>taste</span></a><span>). Our wisdom is story-driven and deeply social. </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>13. Stories, like all meaning, are </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/all-meaning-is-relational" target="_blank"><span>relational</span></a><span> (intrinsically social, not individual). We’re likely the most other-dependent species ever (inalienably </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/you-are-by-nature-self-deficient" target="_blank"><span>self-deficient</span></a><span> by nature). Those with life-structuring stories that are fittest for cooperation, win.</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>14. Our innate </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/our-storied-nature" target="_blank"><span>story-hunger</span></a><span> enables what Rebecca Goldstein calls our “mattering </span><a href="https://www.edge.org/conversation/rebecca_newberger_goldstein-the-mattering-instinct" target="_blank"><span>instinct</span></a><span>.” We’re driven to connect to cooperative projects greater than ourselves. If a community’s life-shaping stories don’t connect mattering to collective </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/economics-vs-fiction-on-human-nature" target="_blank"><span>survival</span></a><span> (and related </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/15-word-fix-for-major-tragic-mistake-needism" target="_blank"><span>needs</span></a><span>), that community, and those stories, won’t </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/prometheus-vs-tragedy-of-the-commons-myth" target="_blank"><span>survive</span></a><span>.</span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>15. The story that evolution is all about competition, overlooks widespread cooperation. Symbiosis isn’t rare, it’s the </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/each-of-you-is-a-multitude-heres-why" target="_blank"><span>rule</span></a><span>. Every “selfish gene” must </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/every-self-gene-must-also-cooperate" target="_blank"><span>cooperate</span></a><span>. Every animal cooperates with billions of </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/each-of-you-is-a-multitude-heres-why" target="_blank"><span>microbiome</span></a><span> mates. Trees run redistributive social </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/wow-trees-have-social-security-and-vast-redistribution" target="_blank"><span>safety nets</span></a><span>. </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>16. These cooperation-and-competition-mixing strategies face natural selection, and the most sustainably productive wins. Internal competition that hinders sustainable cooperation becomes </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/do-we-need-to-rescue-rationality" target="_blank"><span>self-defeating</span></a><span>. Humans are how evolution exceeds the limits of individual </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/are-there-logical-limits-to-self-maximization" target="_blank"><span>competition</span></a><span> and slow-changing </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/the-logic-of-genes-and-genesis" target="_blank"><span>genes.</span></a></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>17. A narrative-level natural selection is at work. Communities with story norms that help suppress destructive internal competition, survive better. History shows victory goes to those who “cooperated </span><a href="https://books.google.com/books?id=DczADQAAQBAJ&pg=PP116&dq=yuval+harari+homo+deus+%22ants+and+bees%22&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwi5v5yTrMXSAhUJCsAKHRgKBewQ6AEIHDAA#v=snippet&q=%22history%20provides%20ample%20evidence%22%20%22Victory%20almost%20invariably%20went%20to%20those%20who%20cooperated%20better%E2%80%9D&f=false" target="_blank"><span>better</span></a><span>.” </span></p> <p dir="ltr"><span>18. It’s clear we can’t live without tools. But it also took large teams and large tales to enable our </span><a href="http://bigthink.com/errors-we-live-by/did-paleo-economics-shape-our-moralities" target="_blank"><span>cooperative</span></a><span> survival and dominance. That’s the bigger story. That’s what matters. </span></p> <p><span> </span></p> <p>-- </p> <p><em>Illustration by <a href="http://juliasuits.net/" target="_blank">Julia Suits</a>, </em>The New Yorker<em> cartoonist & author of </em>The Extraordinary Catalog of Peculiar Inventions</p>
Keep reading
Show less
