fMRI study of dogs suggests they do know human words

"What a shock," said no dog owner ever.

  • Dogs respond neurally in different ways to words they know and words they don't.
  • fMRIs seem to also show how eager dogs are to please.
  • You're not imagining things: They do understand.

It's obvious to dog lovers that we can communicate with our fuzzy pals. Say the words "out," "walk," or "car" and just watch the tail. Still, there could be other cues tipping off our meaning such as our tone of voice or some visual cue we're unconsciously providing. As a result, it's difficult for a non-scientist to know for sure if a dog understands a word or is responding to some other stimulus.

Scientists in Gregory Berns lab at Emory University, however, have learned how to train dogs via positive reinforcement to hold still for fMRI scans. Now a new study has analyzed the neural activity in a group of dogs, leading researchers to what for many dog fans is a satisfying conclusion: Yep, dogs can learn words.

The findings were published October 15 in Frontiers in Neuroscience as "Awake fMRI Reveals Brain Regions for Novel Word Detection in Dogs." According to Ashley Prichard, PhD candidate and lead author of the study, "Many dog owners think that their dogs know what some words mean, but there really isn't much scientific evidence to support that. We wanted to get data from the dogs themselves — not just owner reports."

The study's subjects

The researcher worked with 12 dogs of different breeds and their owners. The dogs had been trained at home to retrieve two specific toys when identified by name — their owners held up each object while saying its name until the dog had it. Golden Retriever/Labrador Eddie, for example, eventually knew what to go get when asked for "Piggy" or "Monkey."

(Prichard, et al)

Eddie, Monkey, and Piggy

The experiments

While the dog's neural activity was being scanned via fMRI, owners said an object's name, and then held up the object in the first phase of the tests. Next, owners held up other objects while saying nonsense words like "bobbu" or "bodmick." The difference in brain activity when responding to known words vs. novel words was significant and exciting, showing definitively that dogs know the difference between words they know and those they don't.

The scientists weren't surprised by this, but there was an intriguing twist, says Prichard: "We expected to see that dogs neurally discriminate between words that they know and words that they don't. What's surprising is that the result is opposite to that of research on humans — people typically show greater neural activation for known words than novel words." The researchers' hypothesis is that this reflects the dogs straining to understand their owners. "Dogs ultimately want to please their owners, and perhaps also receive praise or food," says Berns.

Who's a good boy?

(Prichard, et al)

Velcro, hippo, and ball (Velcro is the pooch)

Activity insights

In half the dogs, the novel words produced increased activity in the brain area similar to our angular gyrus where we process differences in words' meanings.

In the remaining dogs, other areas lit up that scientists believe correspond to the human left temporal cortex and amygdala, the caudate nucleus, and the thalamus. These differences may be down to differences in the breeds, individual dogs, or imperfections in our matching of human to canine brain structures.

In any event, the researchers were able to conclude, "Dogs may have varying capacity and motivation for learning and understanding human words, but they appear to have a neural representation for the meaning of words they have been taught, beyond just a low-level Pavlovian response."

phys.org

Dogs hanging out in Berns' lab for another study

One last thing

Though it's clear dogs do learn words, that doesn't mean words are the best cues for training a dog. Prichard says, "When people want to teach their dog a trick, they often use a verbal command because that's what we humans prefer. From the dog's perspective, however, a visual command might be more effective, helping the dog learn the trick faster."

Big Think Edge
  • The meaning of the word 'confidence' seems obvious. But it's not the same as self-esteem.
  • Confidence isn't just a feeling on your inside. It comes from taking action in the world.
  • Join Big Think Edge today and learn how to achieve more confidence when and where it really matters.
Sponsored by the Institute for Humane Studies
  • There are 2 different approaches to governing free speech on college campuses.
  • One is a morality/order approach. The other is a bottom-up approach.
  • Emily Chamlee-Wright says there are many benefits to having no one central authority on what is appropriate speech.

Is there an optimal time of day to exercise?

Two new studies say yes. Unfortunately, each claims a different time.

Bronx, N.Y.: NYPD officer Julissa Camacho works out at the 44th precinct gym in the Bronx, New York on April 3, 2019. (Photo by Alejandra Villa Loarca/Newsday via Getty Images)
Surprising Science
  • Research at the Weizmann Institute of Sciences declares evening to be the best time for an exercise session.
  • Not so fast, says a new study at UC Irvine, which replies that late morning is the optimal workout time.
  • Both studies involved mice on treadmills and measured different markers to produce their results.
Keep reading Show less
Big Think Edge
  • Economist Sylvia Ann Hewlett breaks down what qualities will inspire others to believe in you.
  • Here's how 300 leaders and 4,000 mid-level managers described someone with executive presence.
  • Get more deep insights like these to power your career forward. Join Big Think Edge.