What is Big Think?  

We are Big Idea Hunters…

We live in a time of information abundance, which far too many of us see as information overload. With the sum total of human knowledge, past and present, at our fingertips, we’re faced with a crisis of attention: which ideas should we engage with, and why? Big Think is an evolving roadmap to the best thinking on the planet — the ideas that can help you think flexibly and act decisively in a multivariate world.

A word about Big Ideas and Themes — The architecture of Big Think

Big ideas are lenses for envisioning the future. Every article and video on bigthink.com and on our learning platforms is based on an emerging “big idea” that is significant, widely relevant, and actionable. We’re sifting the noise for the questions and insights that have the power to change all of our lives, for decades to come. For example, reverse-engineering is a big idea in that the concept is increasingly useful across multiple disciplines, from education to nanotechnology.

Themes are the seven broad umbrellas under which we organize the hundreds of big ideas that populate Big Think. They include New World Order, Earth and Beyond, 21st Century Living, Going Mental, Extreme Biology, Power and Influence, and Inventing the Future.

Big Think Features:

12,000+ Expert Videos

1

Browse videos featuring experts across a wide range of disciplines, from personal health to business leadership to neuroscience.

Watch videos

World Renowned Bloggers

2

Big Think’s contributors offer expert analysis of the big ideas behind the news.

Go to blogs

Big Think Edge

3

Big Think’s Edge learning platform for career mentorship and professional development provides engaging and actionable courses delivered by the people who are shaping our future.

Find out more
Close

A Rainbow Is A Song: The Wild, Curious & Wonderful World of Synesthesia

July 29, 2012, 12:00 AM
Sound%20waves

What's the Big Idea?

On a late winter day in 1922, the sound of a gun shot resounded with a loud boom in the hills surrounding the house of three-year-old Edgar Curtis. The sound itself wasn't out of the ordinary, since the Curtises lived near a firing range. What was extraordinary was the question the boy turned to ask his mother: "What is that big, black noise?" 

A few days later, when his mother was putting him to bed, Edgar heard the chirping of a shrill cricket and demanded, "What is that little white noise?" For Edgar, low, rhythmic notes were dark in color. High-pitched sounds were pale, and, researchers later discovered, tones in between were variously red, blue, and purple. A rainbow was "a song." 

Edgar Curtis' story is an early example in the scientific literature of synesthesia, a neurological phenomenon in which one or more sensory modalities are linked. "There are many different forms,"  says David Eagleman, a neuroscientist known for his ability to garner important insights into the nature of perception and consciousness through idiosyncratic methods. "Essentially, any cross-blending of the senses that you can think of, my colleagues and I have found a case somewhere."  

Watch our interview on synesthesia with neuroscientist David Eagleman:

"It used to be thought this was very rare," says Eagleman. "The original estimates were 1 in 20,000, but we now know it’s quite common. Probably up to 4 percent of the population has some form of synesthesia."

The study of synesthesia has historically been marked by such misconceptions. 19th century romantics and early 20th century surrealists were intrigued, since the impressionistic perceptions of synesthetes seemed to jibe with the generally fluid and emotional sense of reality embraced by serious thinkers of the time. But the rise of behaviorism lead to a dramatic decrease in engagement with the subject. 

In the behaviorist framework, synesthetes were seen as especially sensitive to typical memory associations; nothing more, nothing less. Perhaps five was gold simply because you saw it as a child, painted on the side of a fire truck racing through the streets. The number of publications released per decade on synesthesia dropped from about 135 in the 1920's to about 5 in the 1960's. ("Before brain scans and before recent research into the brain, people were very reluctant to do any studies... it just seemed so wacky," explains Siri Husvedt.)

Today, we understand that synesthesia is internal, automatic, involuntary, and unconscious. Neuroscientists like V.S. Ramachandran and Eagleman have radically advanced our knowledge of the phenomenon, using technology and creativity to show that synesthesia is biologically-based, perceptually real, and distinct from either associations or hallucinations.    

To conduct the first systematic study of synesthesia, Eagleman and his team at the Laboratory for Perception created a self-administered test and published it online at synesthete.org for all to see. The test went viral and gave them instant access to a global pool of synesthetes.

What's the Significance?

Among the most common forms of synesthesia are sound --> color, like Edgar Curtis', and color --> number or grapheme, in which learned sequences such as the alphabet trigger a color experience. Here are the alphabets of two synesthetes, Carol (1) and Karen (2), an artist and language researcher working at MIT

 

Less common, but still prevalent, is the linking of color with touch or taste with touch. For example, Richard E. Cytowic, MD and coauthor with Eagleman of Wednesday Is Indigo Bluerecalls being served dinner by a friend who was embarrassed that the roast chicken he'd made was "too round," despite all his efforts to make it pointy. 

Synesthesia is experienced from birth, so synesthetes are often unaware that there's anything unusual about the way they see the world until it's pointed out to them. To a synesthete, it's just self-evidently true that J is purple, as Eagleman says, or that meat feels sharp, or that August is acid green.

The question is whether the atypical sensory experience is the result of "crossed wiring" in the brain, or if it's the result of a genetically- driven overabundance of neural connections in the brain.

It was recently shown to be heritable, and researchers at the Laboratory for Perception are tracing the genes for it right now. But the consequences of today's explosion of interest in synesthesia has consequences for more than just the lucky few for whom it runs in the family. Says Eagleman:

It's a good inroad into understanding how different brains can be perceive reality differently. So, you’re sitting here, your neighbor is sitting here, and you’re both looking at the same thing -- yet you’re seeing the world very differently...  Most synesthetes, historically, have lived their whole lives and may even die without suspecting that they’re seeing reality differently than someone else, because we all accept the reality presented to us.  So synesthesia is a direct way to look at how individual changes can lead to different beliefs about reality.

Are you a synesthete? Take David Eagleman's test: 

The Synesthesia Battery

 

Image courtesy of Shutterstock.com/agsandrew

 

A Rainbow Is A Song: The Wi...

Newsletter: Share: