Neuroscience shows that speed reading is bullshit

- Most people can read 200–400 words per minute; speed readers claim to manage roughly four times that amount.
- However, research shows that while speed readers have a boosted reading rate, they remember less about what they read compared to regular readers.
- Reading ability must be honed through effort, not gimmicks.
Forty years ago, Donald Homa, a professor of psychology at Arizona State University specializing in memory and the visual perception of linguistic stimuli, was contacted by officials at the American Speed Reading Academy with an extraordinary tale. Two of their pupils had achieved a reading rate in excess of 100,000 words per minute, more than ten times the speed of the Academy’s average student and more than 300 times what a college-educated adult can muster (between 200 and 400 words per minute). Would he be willing to assess their prodigious skills in a laboratory setting?
Curious, Homa happily obliged. In the lab, he tasked the two men with speed reading an entire college-level textbook and then taking a multiple-choice test to gauge their comprehension. After finishing the text in mere minutes, they took the test and absolutely bombed. They hadn’t learned much of anything.
“The only noteworthy skill exhibited by the two speed readers was a remarkable dexterity in page-turning,” Homa concluded.
While this episode is admittedly anecdotal, it does exemplify what scientists have broadly learned about speed reading: It doesn’t work.
Two reasons speed reading doesn’t work
For well over half a century, speed reading courses have promised to drastically accelerate one’s reading ability with no detriment to comprehension. Proponents claim people can become speed readers by learning to take in more words with less eye movement and by silencing their inner speech that accompanies reading.
Summarizing decades of research in an article published in 2016, an all-star team of cognitive scientists and linguists specializing in reading ability and visual perception debunked both of these tenets.
First, how human vision works, including the very structure of the eye, doesn’t allow us to see words on the periphery of our visual field with enough clarity to fully take in their meaning. The notion that whole blocks of words can be understood at a glance is nonsensical. Moreover, experiments have repeatedly shown that speed readers’ peripheral vision is no better than that of a regular reader. It’s something that can’t be leveled up.
Second, silencing one’s inner reading voice can permit one to intake text more quickly; however, it comes at the expense of comprehension. Sounds are key to language, so translating visual information into phonological (sound) form, even just in one’s head, is necessary for complete understanding of written words.
Reading, fast and slow
Even so, proponents of speed reading programs point to data showing that students’ reading speed improves while maintaining comprehension. Students are given a pre- and post-test and seem to experience a sizable improvement; however, the authors of the 2016 report countered that these results are generally bogus.
“Sometimes the pre-test is harder than the post-test, and other times trainees are tested repeatedly on the same material,” they write. “In both cases, it is inevitable that their performance will be better on the post-test merely because of the relative difficulty of the tests or because of repeated exposure.”
When scientists have rigorously scrutinized speed reading courses, they have repeatedly found that students do indeed boost their reading rate, measured in words per minute, but this acceleration comes at the expense of comprehension. The faster people read, the less they remember what they have read.
Nowadays, some apps purport to make speed reading easy. Most of these present a text’s words one at a time in the same spot, allowing the user to adjust the presentation speed. This method, termed “rapid serial visual presentation,” permits the reader’s gaze to remain fixed — eliminating the supposedly wasteful need to scan from word to word. Alas, these apps don’t work either. As users increase the text presentation speed, their comprehension falls.
Take a look, it’s in a book
So if speed reading is bogus, is there any way to read more quickly? Yes, the authors of the 2016 article say.
The answer is to read more, in conjunction with expanding one’s vocabulary. Such practice is admittedly time-consuming, but like so many other skills that require hours and hours of repetition to perfect, reading ability must be honed through effort. Like a pianist playing every day for years, a basketball player taking endless shots, or a pilot spending thousands of hours in the air, expertise takes time. It cannot be taught by way of a 12-week training program.