What is Big Think?  

We are Big Idea Hunters…

We live in a time of information abundance, which far too many of us see as information overload. With the sum total of human knowledge, past and present, at our fingertips, we’re faced with a crisis of attention: which ideas should we engage with, and why? Big Think is an evolving roadmap to the best thinking on the planet — the ideas that can help you think flexibly and act decisively in a multivariate world.

A word about Big Ideas and Themes — The architecture of Big Think

Big ideas are lenses for envisioning the future. Every article and video on bigthink.com and on our learning platforms is based on an emerging “big idea” that is significant, widely relevant, and actionable. We’re sifting the noise for the questions and insights that have the power to change all of our lives, for decades to come. For example, reverse-engineering is a big idea in that the concept is increasingly useful across multiple disciplines, from education to nanotechnology.

Themes are the seven broad umbrellas under which we organize the hundreds of big ideas that populate Big Think. They include New World Order, Earth and Beyond, 21st Century Living, Going Mental, Extreme Biology, Power and Influence, and Inventing the Future.

Big Think Features:

12,000+ Expert Videos

1

Browse videos featuring experts across a wide range of disciplines, from personal health to business leadership to neuroscience.

Watch videos

World Renowned Bloggers

2

Big Think’s contributors offer expert analysis of the big ideas behind the news.

Go to blogs

Big Think Edge

3

Big Think’s Edge learning platform for career mentorship and professional development provides engaging and actionable courses delivered by the people who are shaping our future.

Find out more
Close
With rendition switcher

Transcript

VS Ramachandran:  Well, in turns out, when there’s a small lesion in one part of the brain, what you get is not an across-the-board reduction of all their mental capacity a sort of blunting of your mental functions.  What you see is a highly selective loss of one specific function, which is recognizing faces or reading, and other functions is being preserved intact.  So you can… this gives you some confidence in saying that that region is somehow specialized for that function.  You can then home in on the micro circuitry in that region and see what it’s connected to and understand the function and logic and map function and structure.  So that’s the goal of the whole enterprise. 

So what we do is we look at patients who have sustained injury, look at how their behavior, how their mind has changed and ask ourselves, what can this tell you about the functions of the normal brain.  That’s one agenda, and then there’s the clinical agenda of trying to help the patient and sometimes we succeed and sometimes we don’t.

Capgras Syndrome

One example of this is what’s called Capgras Syndrome. The person says, “This woman looks just like my mother, doctor, but she’s an imposter, she is some other woman pretending to be my mother.”  So there’s a strong emotional component to it.  Sometimes it acquires a little paranoid flavor.  Why is this woman following all the time, she’s not my mother, she looks like my mother, but she is not.  Why is she pretending to be my mother? 

So what causes this? What we think is going on here is the visual pathways are normal, the fusiform face gyrus is normal, that’s why he can recognize his mother, and say it looks like my mother.  But the wire that goes from there to the amygdala is cut by the accident.  Therefore, he looks at his mother and there’s no emotional arousal.  So there’s no warmth when he looks at him mother or no fear when he looks at his mother.  So he says, “If this is my mom, how come I’m not feeling anything?  It’s might be some other woman, it’s an imposter.”  The only sensible interpretation in his brain… it seems bizarre to you and me, but given the disconnection in his brain between vision and emotion, vision is normal, emotion is normal, but the two are uncoupled.  The only sensible interpretation is this very bizarre interpretation that she is an imposter.  

But if you come up with that anatomical explanation, you solved the mystery, you do a Sherlock Holmes like deduction here, and then you analyze what is going on and it’s mystery solved.  And you can test it experimentally using a very simple procedure.  

Cotard's Disorder

Another even more bizarre disorder, if you can believe it, is Cotard’s Disorder, where a person says, “I am not alive; I am dead.  I am a rotting dead body.  I’ve been buried.  I’m a corpse; I can smell my stinking corpse.”  

Now, why does Cotard's occur?  We don’t know.  But it is a representation of self just like there’s a representation of other people in your brain.  And when there is an alienation of the self because of some kind of disconnection going on in the brain, it may contribute to Cotard's Syndrome.  But also, just at in Capgras Syndrome, there’s a disconnection between feasible sensory signals in the amygdala and the limbic structures so that your mother fails to evoke emotion, imagine a much more widespread disconnection of all sensory input; hearing, touch, vision, everything is disconnected from the limbic structures.  It's an impoverished sense of self, an alienation of self.  None of the sensations of the external world have any emotional significance that’s about as close as you can get to existential annihilation.

More from the Big Idea for Sunday, September 25 2011

 

The Most Bizarre Brain Diso...

Newsletter: Share: